Evolving or unchanged? Investigating perceptual profiles over time in psychosis and comparing with healthy controls
There is a growing body of evidence indicating altered perceptual profiles in psychosis, which can be assessed by evaluating exteroceptive (e.g., auditory and visual) and interoceptive (e.g., heartbeat and breathing) sensory processing through self-administered questionnaires. However, the unexplored question remains whether perceptual alterations depend on the phase of illness. In this observational, STROBE-compliant study, we adopted the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA; eight domains), Adult/Adolescent Sensory Profile (AASP; four domains), and Positive and Negative Symptoms Scale (PANSS) to measure interoception, exteroception, and symptom severity, respectively. Our primary aim was to compare MAIA, AASP, and PANSS differences in patients with psychosis (PSY patients) between post-acute (PSY-T1) and remission phases (PSY-T2). Student’s t-tests or Wilcoxon paired-sample tests were used based on the variable distribution. MAIA/AASP data were also collected from healthy controls (HCs). As a secondary aim, we examined MAIA/AASP differences between PSY-T1/T2 and HC using MANOVA and Mann–Whitney tests. Bonferroni–Holm correction was implemented. Data were collected from 23 PSY patients (55% females; mean age: 38.35 ± 12.46 years) and 210 HC (46% females; mean age: 39.81 ± 13.78 years). No differences were found between PSY-T1 and PSY-T2 (Bonferroni–Holm P > 0.05) for MAIA/AASP scores, while PANSS total and positive scores were higher in PSY-T1 compared to PSY-T2, with Bonferroni–Holm p-values of 0.032 and 0.045, respectively. Although MAIA/AASP domains (noticing/body listening and low registration) were increased in PSY-T1 compared to HC, no differences were observed between PSY-T2 and HC. The heterogeneous results in the literature regarding perceptual profiles should be contextualized by considering that fluctuations in patients with psychosis can become significant when compared to the general population.
- Merlau-Ponty M, 1945, Phenomenology of Perception. London: Routledge.
- Gallagher S, 2000, Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci, 4: 14–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01417-5
- Kean C, 2009, Silencing the self: Schizophrenia as a self-disturbance. Schizophr Bull, 35: 1034–1036. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp043
- Parnas J, 2013, On Psychosis: Karl Jaspers and Beyond. Vol. 1. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Available from: https:// academic.oup.com/book/25034/chapter/189090895
- Kraepelin E, 1904, Psychiatrie. New York: Macmillan.
- Parnas J, Møller P, Kircher T, et al., 2005, EASE: Examination of anomalous self-experience. Psychopathology, 38: 236–258. https://doi.org/10.1159/000088441
- Raballo A, Poletti M, Preti A, et al., 2021, The self in the spectrum: A meta-analysis of the evidence linking basic self-disorders and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull, 47: 1007–1017. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbaa201
- Brown C, Cromwell RL, Filion D, et al., 2002, Sensory processing in schizophrenia: Missing and avoiding information. Schizophr Res, 55: 187–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00255-9
- Mcghie A, Chapman J, 1961, Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia. Br J Med Psychol, 34: 103–116. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1961.tb00936.x
- Galderisi S, Maj M, Mucci A, et al., 2002, Historical, psychopathological, neurological, and neuropsychological aspects of deficit schizophrenia: A multicenter study. Am J Psychiatry, 159: 983–990. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.6.983
- Postmes L, Sno HN, Goedhart S, et al., 2014, Schizophrenia as a self-disorder due to perceptual incoherence. Schizophr Res, 152: 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.027
- Conrad K, 1958, Die Beginnende Schizophrenie. Stuttgart: Thieme Group. p. 1595–1598.
- Tatossian A, 1979, La Phénoménologie des Psychoses. France: Le Cercle Herméneutique.
- Morenz R, Woolverton C, Frost RB, et al., 2015, Clinical correlates of distorted auditory perception in first-episode psychosis. Early Interv Psychiatry, 9: 248–251. https://doi.org/10.1111/eip.12150
- Van der Weiden A, Prikken M, van Haren NEM, 2015, Self-other integration and distinction in schizophrenia: A theoretical analysis and a review of the evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 57: 220–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.004
- Mishara AL, Sterzer P, 2015, Phenomenology is bayesian in its application to delusions. World Psychiatry, 14: 184–185. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20213
- Parham LD, Roush S, Downing DT, et al., 2019, Sensory characteristics of youth at clinical high risk for psychosis. Early Interv Psychiatry, 13: 264–271. https://doi.org/10.1111/eip.12475
- Brown C, Tollefson N, Dunn W, et al., 2001, The adult sensory profile: Measuring patterns of sensory processing. Am J Occup Ther, 55: 75–82. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.55.1.75
- De Boer JN, Linszen MMJ, De Vries J, et al., 2019, Auditory hallucinations, top-down processing and language perception: A general population study. Psychol Med, 49: 2772–2780. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171800380X
- Mehling WE, Price C, Daubenmier JJ, et al., 2012, The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA). PLoS One, 7: e48230. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048230
- Van den Boogert F, Klein K, Spaan P, et al., 2022, Sensory processing difficulties in psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Res, 151: 173–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.04.020
- Mehling WE, Acree M, Stewart A, et al., 2018, The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness, version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS One, 13: e0208034. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208034
- Montoya-Hurtado O, Gómez-Jaramillo N, Bermúdez- Jaimes G, et al., 2023, Psychometric properties of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA) questionnaire in Colombian University students. J Clin Med, 12: 2937. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12082937
- Von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, et al., 2007, The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. Lancet, 370: 1453–1457. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61602-X
- Kay SR, Flszbeln A, Qpjer LA, 1987, The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull, 13: 261–276. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/13.2.261
- Gardner DM, Murphy AL, O’Donnell H, et al., 2010, International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. Am J Psychiatry, 167: 686–693. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060802
- Hayasaka Y, Purgato M, Magni LR, et al., 2015, Dose equivalents of antidepressants: Evidence-based recommendations from randomized controlled trials. J Affect Disord, 180: 179–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.03.021
- Inada T, Inagaki A, 2015, Psychotropic dose equivalence in Japan. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci, 69: 440–447. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12275
- Koreki A, Funayama M, Terasawa Y, et al., 2021, Aberrant interoceptive accuracy in patients with schizophrenia performing a heartbeat counting task. Schizophr Bull Open, 2: sgaa067. https://doi.org/10.1093/schizbullopen/sgaa067
- Torregrossa LJ, Amedy A, Roig J, et al., 2022, Interoceptive functioning in schizophrenia and schizotypy. Schizophr Res, 239: 151–159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2021.11.046
- Trevisan DA, Mehling WE, McPartland JC, 2021, Adaptive and maladaptive bodily awareness: Distinguishing interoceptive sensibility and interoceptive attention from anxiety-induced somatization in autism and alexithymia. Autism Res, 14: 240–247. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2458
- Khalsa SS, Adolphs R, Cameron OG, et al., 2018, Interoception and mental health: A roadmap. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging, 3: 501–513. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004
- De Jong M, Lazar SW, Hug K, et al., 2016, Effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on body awareness in patients with chronic pain and comorbid depression. Front Psychol, 7: 967. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00967
- Valenzuela-Moguillansky C, Reyes-Reyes A, Gaete MI, 2017, Exteroceptive and interoceptive body-self awareness in fibromyalgia patients. Front Hum Neurosci, 11: 117. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00117
- Mul CL, Stagg SD, Herbelin B, et al., 2018, The feeling of me feeling for you: Interoception, alexithymia and empathy in autism. J Autism Dev Disord, 48: 2953–2967. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3564-3
- Williams ZJ, Suzman E, Bordman SL, et al., 2023, Characterizing interoceptive differences in autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis of case-control studies. J Autism Dev Disord, 53: 947–962. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-022-05656-2
- Rosner B, Glynn RJ, Lee ML, 2006, The Wilcoxon signed rank test for paired comparisons of clustered data. Biometrics, 62: 185–192. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1541-0420.2005.00389.x
- Hsu H, Lachenbruch PA, 2014, Paired t test. In: Wiley StatsRef: Statistics Reference Online. Hoboken: Wiley.
- Van Aelst S, Willems G, 2011, Robust and efficient one-way MANOVA tests. J Am Stat Assoc, 106: 706–718.
- McKnight PE, Najab J, 2009, Mann-whitney U Test. In: The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. Hoboken: John Wiley and Sons.
- Abdi H, 2010, Holm’s sequential bonferroni procedure. In: Encyclopedia of Research Design. Thousand Oaks (CA): Sage. Available from: https://www.utd.edu/~herve
- Dudley R, Taylor P, Wickham S, et al., 2016, Psychosis, delusions and the “jumping to conclusions” reasoning bias: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull, 42: 652–665. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv150
- Studer RK, Negrete H, Civen PA, et al., 1995, Protein kinase C signals thromboxane induced increases in fibronectin synthesis and TGF-beta bioactivity in mesangial cells. Kidney Int, 48: 422–430. https://doi.org/10.1038/ki.1995.310
- Howes OD, Nour MM, 2016, Dopamine and the aberrant salience hypothesis of schizophrenia. World Psychiatry, 15: 3–4. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20276
- Yao B, Thakkar K, 2022, Interoception abnormalities in schizophrenia: A review of preliminary evidence and an integration with bayesian accounts of psychosis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 132: 757–773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.016
- Seth AK, 2013, Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends Cogn Sci, 17: 565–573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007
- Allen M, Tsakiris M, 2018, The body as first prior: Interoceptive predictive processing and the primacy of self-models. In: The Interoceptive Mind: From Homeostasis to Awareness. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 27–45. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/book/9637/chapter/156706805
- Bretas R, Tia B, Yamazaki Y, et al., 2022, Multisensory integration and belief in the self. Front Psychol, 13: 983592. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.983592
- Burns CO, Dixon DR, Novack M, et al., 2017, A systematic review of assessments for sensory processing abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder. Rev J Autism Dev Disord, 4: 209–224. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-017-0109-1